Skip to main content

Lab #2 - Image formation in a pinhole camera

Lab #2 - Image formation in a pinhole camera

To help you understand how your eye helps you organize the patterns of light available in the environment, the exercises in Lab #2 involve building a simple model of the eye called a pinhole camera. By making this model, you'll be able to observe how image formation happens in an eye and how you can manipulate different qualities of the image by changing features of the model.

You can find the document describing the lab exercises at the following link:

https://drive.google.com/file/d/1hlqnJT5pdG6A6n2dc6fYI4eJ5gLBiPbU/view


When you're done, move on to the next post so we can discuss what you see with a pinhole camera and what it tells us about how real eyes work.

Comments

Popular posts from this blog

Monocular cues for depth perception

Monocular cues for depth perception In our last post, we discussed how you can use the information from your two eyes to estimate the relative depth of objects in the visual field. Both vergence and binocular disparity provided cues to where objects were situated in depth relative some fixation point, allowing us to obtain some information about the 3D arrangement of objects in space. Clearly, two eyes are helpful in resolving the ambiguity that follows from projecting the light coming from a three-dimensional scene onto a two-dimensional surface. We said that this projection of light onto the retina made it much harder to make good guesses about depth from one image, and that using two images was a necessary step towards making it possible for you to recover this information from the retinal data. However, consider the picture below:   Figure 1 - A boid in some trees. Some things here look closer to you than others, but how do you tell that when you don't have bino...

Observing the retina (and what it can do)

Observing the retina (and what it can do) Now that we’ve seen how images are formed inside of a pinhole camera, we have a sense of how patterns of light from the environment become patterns of light inside the eye. The next question is how those patterns become signals that can be sent from the eye to the brain. This process is called transduction , and within the eye, the structure that actually transduces light is called the retina . How does this bit of tissue sense light? Something must be happening that turns light into an electrical signal, but what? We’ll develop a quantitative model of how this works, but first, we’ll try to develop a basic understanding of the retina based on some simple observations. Compared to some of our previous discussions, this is going to be a little trickier – the retina is inside our eye, for example, so we can’t just look at the parts of it the way you were able to look at your own pupil. Instead, we’re going to adopt a dual strategy of (1) Makin...

Introduction to high-level vision

Introduction to high-level vision With this blog post, we begin to talk about a new and challenging topic in human vision: high-level vision. Before, we go any further, we need to address an important question: How is this different than what we’ve been talking about before?   To date, we’ve spent a lot of time talking about some of the basic principles regarding the way your visual system measures the light in your environment. For example, you have photoreceptors that transduce light and provide a signal that carries information about the wavelength of the light you saw. Further on in your visual system, cells in the LGN and V1 measure various patterns of light and send information on about the dots, edges, and lines you may have seen. These kinds of processes that are mostly concerned with making measurements that describe the incoming light are what I’d refer to as low-level vision . Figure 1  - Measuring the wavelength content of a light or the pattern of li...